Skip to main content

About Peggy DesAutels

Most moral theory is impossible to apply to our actual lives. In my research, I argue that a good moral theory should provide useful norms for our day-to-day moral perceptions, deliberations, choices, and practices. It should accommodate human cognitive constraints and respond to the complexities of actual experienced lives. In other words, our moral theories should be psychologically and socially realistic. Our moral theories and norms must also be responsive to the fact that we live in a society in which some are systematically disadvantaged because of their gender, race, or class.
In order to provide realistic and non-idealized moral norms, I draw on contemporary moral theory, feminist theory, and advances in social psychology, cognitive science and neuroscience. I have published three co-edited volumes: Feminists Doing Ethics, edited with Joanne Waugh; Moral Psychology: Feminist Ethics and Social Theory, edited with Margaret Urban Walker; and Global Feminist Ethics, edited with Rebecca Whisnant.
My recently published articles and book chapters are also committed to a non-idealized approach to moral theorizing. In my piece, "Resisting Organizational Power" (in Tessman’s Feminist Ethics and Social and Political Theory: Theorizing the Non-Ideal), I stress that there is seldom an ideal moral outcome for whistleblowers, since in many cases whistleblowers become psychologically devastated and morally incapacitated from doing "the right thing." This is even more likely to be the case for women whistleblowers. In “Moral Mindfulness,” I emphasize that even those with the best of moral intentions may miss that a situation calls for a moral response. I draw on research in social psychology to show that we often fail to notice moral situations, because our thoughts and behaviors are influenced subconsciously or overtly by social contexts. In "Musings: Experimental Philosophy," I use humor to make a serious criticism of philosophers’ recent attempts to incorporate classroom thought experiments and their students’ “intuitions” into their own philosophic writings. And in "Sex Differences and Neuroethics," I examine recent neuroscience research on sex differences and recent research on what brains are doing when making moral judgments. The result is what I term "feminist neuroethics." I point out that differing hormones in men and women may result in differing ways of incorporating emotions into moral judgments. But I also emphasize that emotional and cognitive differences between men and women are slight and merely “on-average” differences, if they exist at all. Finally, my piece entitled, "Is the Climate any Warmer for Women in Philosophy," combines my interests in cognitive science and philosophy in a slightly different way. It explores why only twenty-one percent of tenure-line faculty in philosophy are women. Are women’s brains less fit for philosophical thinking? Or is something within the culture and practice of philosophy contributing to the low numbers of women in the field? Because there is no neurological evidence that women’s brains are less capable than men’s of doing philosophy, I argue that we need good demographic and climate data in order to identify what aspects of the philosophic culture most need to be addressed and change.
I recently served as a social science research leader on a National Science Foundation (NSF) ADVANCE LEADER grant. This $2.5 million five-year grant was awarded to the consortium consisting of University of Dayton, Wright State University, Central State University, and Air Force Institute of Technology (AFIT). It was intended to promote institutional change to better recruit and advance women in Science, Technology, Engineering, and Math (STEM) fields. My role in this grant was to train equity advisors and educate STEM chairs and faculty throughout the consortium on implicit bias and on best practices tied to recruiting, retaining, and advancing STEM women faculty. I was part of the social science research team for this grant (researching best practices for recruiting and advancing STEM women faculty). I drew on social science research, neuroscience, and feminist theory in my presentations and writings (e.g. “Cases: Where are the Women”) in this area.

Positions

2001 - Present Professor, University of Dayton Department of Philosophy
to

Curriculum Vitae


Disciplines



$
to
Enter a valid date range.

to
Enter a valid date range.

Honors and Awards

  • Recipient of a Frances Elvidge Post-Doctoral Fellowship in Ethics at The Ethics Center of the University of South Florida, St. Petersburg, Florida, 1995–1996.
  • Recipient of a Mr. And Mrs. Spencer T. Olin Fellowship for Women, Washington University, St. Louis, Missouri, 1990–1994.
  • Grand Marshall for recipients of Masters of Arts Degrees in the School of Arts and Sciences, Washington University, Graduation, 1993.
  • Winner of the 14th Helen Stenner Memorial Essay Competition, a $300 prize offered each year to the best graduate student paper written in Washington University’s Philosophy Department, refereed by outside judges, 1991–1992.
  • Grand Marshall for recipients of Masters of Science Degrees in the School of Engineering, Washington University, Graduation, 1985.
  • Graduated with Highest Honors from Principia College, 1977.

Education

to
1995 PhD, Washington University
to
1993 MA, Washington University
to
1988 MS, Washington University
to
1977 BA, Principia College
to

Contact Information

Phone: 937-229-1344


Grants (7)

Articles (8)

Book chapters (4)

Books (4)

No Subject Area (2)