Skip to main content

Individual Differences in Nonhuman Animals: Examining Boredom, Curiosity, and Creativity

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Personality in Nonhuman Animals

Abstract

Personality psychology has traditionally focused on the study of individual differences in human cognition and behavior. More recently, the topic of individual differences in nonhuman animal behavior has featured more prominently in biology and psychology research. The study of individual differences in nonhuman animals has important implications for ecology, conservation, comparative psychology, agriculture, and the care of animals in zoological facilities. Individual differences in animal boredom will be examined in this chapter to highlight the importance of studying variation in boredom proneness and coping styles in nonhuman animals. The negative affective state of boredom is adaptive because it serves as motivation for an individual to re-engage with the environment, a process that can involve curiosity and creativity. Future research on animal boredom should investigate the behavioral and physiological correlates of boredom at both a species and an individual level in order to expand the existing literature and contribute to the future of animal welfare.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 99.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 129.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 199.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Adamec, R. (1975). Behavioral and epileptic determinants of predatory attack behavior in the cat. Canadian Journal of Neurological Sciences, 2, 457–466.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Alligood, C., & Leighty, K. (2015). Putting the “E” in SPIDER: Evolving trends in the evaluation of environmental enrichment efficacy in zoological settings. Animal Behavior and Cognition, 2, 200–217.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baird, R. W. (2000). The killer whale. In H. Whitehead & J. Mann (Eds.), Cetacean societies: Field studies of dolphins and whales (pp. 127–153). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bench, S., & Lench, H. (2013). On the function of boredom. Behavioral Sciences, 3, 459–472. doi:10.3390/bs3030459

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Benson-Amran, S., & Holekamp, K. (2012). Innovative problem solving by wild spotted hyenas. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 279, 4087–4095.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Biondi, L. M., Bó, M. S., & Vassallo, A. I. (2010). Inter-individual and age differences in exploration, neophobia and problem-solving ability in a Neotropical raptor (Milvago chimango). Animal Cognition, 13, 701–710. doi:10.1007/s10071-010-0319-8

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Boesch, C. (2007). What makes us human (Homo sapiens)? The challenge of cognitive cross-species comparison. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 121, 227–240.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Boesch, C. (2008). Taking development and ecology seriously when comparing cognition: Reply to Tomasello and Call (2008). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 122, 453–455.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Buirski, P., Plutchik, R., & Kellerman, H. (1978). Sex differences, dominance, and personality in the chimpanzee. Animal Behaviour, 26, 123–129.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Burghardt, G. (2014). A brief glimpse at the long evolutionary history of play. Animal Behavior and Cognition, 1, 90–98.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Call, J. (2015). Conservatism versus innovation: The great ape story. In A. Kaufman & J. Kaufman (Eds.), Animal creativity and innovation (pp. 397–414). New York: Elsevier.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Cannon, T., Heistermann, M., Hankison, S., Hockings, K., & McLennan, M. (2016). Tailored enrichment strategies and stereotypic behavior in captive individually housed macaques (Macaca spp.). Journal of Applied. Animal Welfare. doi:10.1080/10888705.2015.1126785

    Google Scholar 

  • Chamove, A. S., Eysenck, H. J., & Harlow, H. F. (1972). Personality in monkeys: Factor analyses of rhesus social behavior. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 24, 496–504.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Charmoy, K., Sullivan, T., & Miller, L. J. (2015). Impact of different forms of environmental enrichment on foraging and activity levels in gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla). Animal Behavior and Cognition, 2, 233–240.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coleman, K., & Wilson, D. (1998). Shyness and boldness in pumpkinseed sunfish: individual differences are context-specific. Animal Behavior, 56, 927–936.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Collins, R. P., Litman, J. A., & Spielberger, C. D. (2004). The measurement of perceptual curiosity. Personality and Individual Differences, 36, 1127–1141. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(03)00205-8

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cussen, V., & Mench, J. (2015). The relationship between personality dimensions and resiliency to environmental stress in orange-winged Amazon parrots (Amazona amazonica), as indicated by the development of abnormal behaviors. PLoS ONE, 10, 1–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Daniels, L., Tze, V., & Goetz, T. (2015). Examining boredom: Different causes for different coping profiles. Learning and Individual Differences, 37, 255–261.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dall, S., Houston, A., & McNamara, J. (2004). The behavioural ecology of personality: Consistent individual differences from an adaptive perspective. Ecology Letters, 7, 734–739.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dallaire, J. A., Meagher, R. K., & Mason, G. J. (2012). Individual differences in stereotypic behaviour predict individual differences in the nature and degree of enrichment use in caged American mink. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 142, 98–108. doi:10.1016/j.applanim.2012.09.012

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Darwin, C. (1871/1964). The descent of man, and selection in relation to sex. London: John Murray.

    Google Scholar 

  • Darwin, C. (1872). The expressions of the emotions in man and animals. London: John Murray.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Dellu, F., Mayo, W., Piazza, P. V. V., Le Moal, M., & Simon, H. (1993). Individual differences in behavioral responses to novelty in rats. Possible relationship with the sensation-seeking trait in man. Personality and Individual Differences, 15, 411–418. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(93)90069-F

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Digman, J. M. (1990). Personality structure: Emergence of the five-factor model. Annual Review of Psychology, 41, 417–440.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eastwood, J. D., Frischen, A., Fenske, M. J., & Smilek, D. (2012). The unengaged mind: Defining boredom in terms of attention. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7, 482–495. doi:10.1177/1745691612456044

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eskelinen, H. C., Winship, K. A., & Borger-Turner, J. L. (2015). Sex, age, and individual differences in bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) in response to environmental enrichment. Animal Behavior and Cognition, 2, 241–253. doi:10.12966/abc.08.04.2015

    Google Scholar 

  • Farmer, R., & Sundberg, N. (1986). Boredom proneness—The development and correlates of a new scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 50, 4–17. doi:10.1207/s15327752jpa5001_2

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Found, R., & St. Clair, C. (2016). Behavioural syndromes predict loss of migration in wild elk. Animal Behaviour, 115, 35–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fureix, C., & Meagher, R. K. (2015). What can inactivity (in its various forms) reveal about affective states in nonhuman animals? A review. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 171, 8–24. doi:10.1016/j.applanim.2015.08.036

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Giraldeau, L.-A., & Dubois, F. (2008). Social foraging and the study of exploitative behavior. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 38, 59–104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goetz, T., Frenzel, A., Hall, N., Nett, U., Pekrun, R., & Lipnevich, A. (2014). Types of boredom: An experience sampling approach. Motivation and Emotion, 38, 401–419.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldberg, L. R. (1990). An alternative “description of personality” : The big-five factor structure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 1216–1229.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goldberg, L. R. (1993). The structure of phenotypic personality traits. American Psychologist, 48, 26–34.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gosling, S. (2001). From mice to men: What can we learn about personality from animal research? Psychological Bulletin, 127, 45–86.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gottlieb, D., Capitanio, J., & McCowan, B. (2013). Risk factors for stereotypic behavior and self-biting in Rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta): Animal’s history, current environment, and personality. American Journal of Primatology, 75, 995–1008.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Greene, W., Melillo-Sweeting, K., & Dudzinski, K. (2011). Comparing object play in captive and wild dolphins. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 24, 292–306.

    Google Scholar 

  • Groothuis, T. G. G., & Carere, C. (2005). Avian personalities: Characterization and epigenesis. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 29, 137–150. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.06.010

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Guenther, A., Brust, V., Dersen, M., & Trillmich, F. (2013). Learning and personality types are related in cavies (Cavia aperea). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 128, 1–8.

    Google Scholar 

  • Herrmann, E., Hernández-Lloreda, M. V., Call, J., Hare, B., & Tomasello, M. (2010). The structure of individual differences in the cognitive abilities of children and chimpanzees. Psychological Science, 21, 102–110.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Highfill, L., & Kuczaj, S. (2010). How studies of wild and captive dolphins contribute to our understanding of individual differences and personality. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 23, 269–277.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hogan, L. A., Johnston, S. D., Lisle, A., Horsup, A. B., Janssen, T., & Phillips, C. J. C. (2010). Stereotypies and environmental enrichment in captive southern hairy-nosed wombats, Lasiorhinus latifrons. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 126, 85–95. doi:10.1016/j.applanim.2010.05.009

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hogan, L. A., & Tribe, A. (2007). Prevalence and cause of stereotypic behaviour in common wombats (Vombatus ursinus) residing in Australian zoos. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 105, 180–191. doi:10.1016/j.applanim.2006.06.006

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hopkins, W., Russel, J., & Schaeffer, J. (2014). Chimpanzee intelligence is heritable. Current Biology, 24, 1649–1652.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hoy, J. M., Murray, P. J., & Tribe, A. (2010). Thirty years later: Enrichment practices for captive mammals. Zoo Biology, 29, 303–316. doi:10.1002/zoo.20254

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hunter, S. A., Bay, M. S., Martin, M. L., & Hatfield, J. S. (2002). Behavioral effects of environmental enrichment on harbor seals (Phoca vitulina concolor) and gray seals (Halichoerus grypus). Zoo Biology, 21, 375–387. doi:10.1002/zoo.10042

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huntingford, F., Andrew, G., Mackenzie, S., Morera, D., Coyle, S., Pilarczyk, M., et al. (2010). Coping strategies in a strongly schooling fish, the common carp Cyprinus carpio. Journal of Fish Biology, 76, 1576–1591.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ijichi, C., Collins, L., & Elwood, R. (2013). Evidence for the role of personality in stereotypy predisposition. Animal Behaviour, 85, 1145–1151.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Izzo, G. N., Bashaw, M. J., & Campbell, J. B. (2011). Enrichment and individual differences affect welfare indicators in squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 125, 347.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Joshi, S., & Pillay, N. (2016). Association between personality and stereotypic behavior in the African striped mouse Rhabdomys dilectus. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 174, 154–161.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kennedy, J. (1992). The new antrhopomorphism. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • King, J., Weiss, A., & Sisco, M. (2008). Aping humans- age and sex effects in chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) and human (Homo sapiens) personality. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 122, 418–427.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kuczaj, S.A. II (2017). Animal creativity and innovation. In J. Call, G. Burghardt, I. Pepperberg, C. Snowdon, & T. Zentall (Eds.), APA handbook of comparative psychology : Perception, learning, and cognition. Washington, DC, US: American Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuczaj, S., & Eskelinen, H. (2014a). The “creative dolphin” revisited: What do dolphins do when asked to vary their behavior? Animal Behavior and Cognition, 1, 66–77.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuczaj, S. A., & Eskelinen, H. C. (2014b). Why do Dolphins Play? Animal Behavior and Cognition, 2, 113. doi:10.12966/abc.05.03.2014

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuczaj, S., Highfill, L., & Byerly, H. (2012). The importance of considering context in the assessment of personality characteristics: Evidence from ratings of dolphin personality. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 25, 309–329.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuczaj, S., Lacinak, T., Garver, A., & Scarpuzzi, M. (1998). Can animals enrich their own environment? In V. Hare & K. Worley (Eds.), Proceedings of the third international conference on environmental enrichment (pp. 168–170). San Diego, CA: Shape of Enrichment Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuczaj, S., Makecha, R., Trone, M., Paulos, R., & Ramos, J. (2006). Role of peers in cultural innovation and cultural transmission: Evidence from the play of dolphin calves. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 19, 223–240.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuczaj, S. A., II, & Walker, R. T. (2012). Dolphin problem solving. In T. Zentall & E. Wasserman (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of comparative cognition (pp. 736–756). London: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laule, G., Bloomsmith, M., & Schapiro, S. (2003). The use of positive reinforcement training techniques to enhance the care, management, and welfare of primates in the laboratory. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science, 6, 163–173.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lopes, M., Borger-Turner, J., Eskelinen, H., & Kuczaj, S. (2016). The influence of age, sex, and social affiliation on the responses of bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) to a novel stimulus over time. Animal Behavior and Cognition, 3, 32–45.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mann, S., & Cadman, R. (2014). Does being bored make us more creative? Creativity Research Journal, 26, 165–173. doi:10.1080/10400419.2014.901073

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Markowitz, H., & Aday, C. (1998). Power for captive animals: Contingencies and nature. In D. J. Shepherdson, J. D. Mellen, & M. Hutchins (Eds.), Second nature: Environmental enrichment for captive animals (pp. 47–58). Washington, DC: Smithsonian Institution Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mason, G., Clubb, R., Latham, N., & Vickery, S. (2007). Why and how should we use environmental enrichment to tackle stereotypic behaviour? Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 102, 163–188. doi:10.1016/j.applanim.2006.05.041

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meagher, R. K., & Mason, G. J. (2012). Environmental enrichment reduces signs of boredom in caged mink. PLoS ONE. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0049180

    Google Scholar 

  • Mellen, J., & Sevenich MacPhee, M. (2001). Philosophy of environmental enrichment: Past, present, and future. Zoo Biology, 20, 211–226.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morand-Ferron, J., & Giraldeau, L. A. (2010). Learning behaviorally stable solutions to producer-scrounger games. Behavioral Ecology, 21, 343–348.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Owen, M., Swaisgood, R., Czekala, N., & Lindburg, D. (2005). Enclosure choice and well-being in giant pandas: Is it all about control? Zoo Biology, 24, 475–481.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, E., Worlein, J., Lee, G., Dettmer, A., Varner, E., & Novak, M. (2016). Rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) with self-injurious behavior show less behavioral anxiety during the human intruder test. American Journal of Primatology, 79, 1–8.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pervin, L., & John, O. P. (1997). Personality: Theory and research (7th ed.). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pryor, K. (2015). Creating creative animals. In A. Kaufman & J. Kaufman (Eds.), Animal creativity and innovation (pp. 483–498). New York: Elsevier.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Reader, S. M. (2015). Causes of individual differences in animal exploration and search. Topics in Cognitive Science. doi:10.1111/tops.12148

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Réale, D., Reader, S. M., Sol, D., McDougall, P. T., & Dingemanse, N. J. (2007). Integrating animal temperament within ecology and evolution. Biological Reviews, 82, 291–318.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Renner, M. J. (1987). Experience-dependent changes in exploratory behavior in the adult rat (Rattus norvegicus): Overall activity level and interactions with objects. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 101, 94–100. doi:10.1037//0735-7036.101.1.94

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Renner, M. J., Bennett, A. J., & White, J. C. (1992). Age and sex as factors influencing spontaneous exploration and object investigation by preadult rats (Rattus norvegicus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 106, 217–227. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.106.3.217

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ross, S. (2006). Issues of choice and control in the behaviour of a pair of captive polar bears (Ursus maritimus). Behavioural Processes, 73, 117–120.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Seligman, M. (1972). Learned helplessness. Annual Review of Medicine, 23, 407–412. doi:10.1146/annurev.me.23.020172.002203

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sih, A., & Bell, A. (2008). Insights for behavioral ecology from behavioral syndromes. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 38, 227.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Sih, A., Bell, A., Johnson, J., & Ziemba, R. (2004). Behavioral syndromes: An ecological and evolutionary overview. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 19, 372–378.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sol, D., Griffin, A. S., & Bartomeus, I. (2012). Consumer and motor innovation in the common myna: The role of motivation and emotional responses. Animal Behaviour, 83, 179–188. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.10.024

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stevenson-Hinde, J., & Zunz, M. (1978). Subjective assessment of individual rhesus monkeys. Primates, 19, 473–482.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Swaisgood, R., & Shepardson, D. (2006). Environmental enrichment as a stratedgy for mitigating stereotypies in zoo animals: A literature review and meta-analysis. In G. Mason & J. Rushen (Eds.), Stereotypic animal behaviour: Fundamentals and applications to welfare. Wallingford: CAB International.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tarou, L., & Bashaw, M. (2007). Maximizing the effectiveness of environmental enrichment: Suggestions from the experimental analysis of behavior. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 102, 189–204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tetley, C., & O’Hara, S. (2012). Ratings of animal personality as a tool for improving the breeding, management and welfare of zoo animals. Animal Welfare, 21, 463–476.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thelen, E., & Farish, D. (1977). An analysis of the grooming behavior of wild and mutant strains of Bracon Hebet or (Braconidae: Hymenoptera). Behaviour, 62, 70–102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., & Call, J. (2007). Assessing the validity of ape-human comparisons: A reply to Boesch (2007). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 122, 449–452.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Veissier, I., Boissy, A., Désiré, L., & Greiveldinger, L. (2009). Animals’ emotions: Studies in sheep using appraisal theories. Animal Welfare, 18, 347–354.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vodanovich, S. (2003). Psychometric measures of boredom: A review of the literature. The Journal of Psychology: Interdisciplinary and Applied, 137, 569–595.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vonk, J., & Povinelli, D. (2011). Individual differences in long-term cognitive testing in a group of chimpanzees. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 24, 137–167.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, B. (1992). Stereotypies and attentiveness to novel stimuli: A test in polar bears. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 33, 381–388. doi:10.1016/S0168-1591(05)80074-7

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weiss, A., & King, J. (2015). Great ape origins of personality maturation and sex differences: A study of orangutans and chimpanzees. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 108, 648–664.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wemelsfelder, F. (1984). Animal Boredom : Is a scientific study of the subjective experiences of animals possible ? In M. W. Fox & L. D. Mickley (Eds.), Advances in animal welfare science, vol. 85 (pp. 115–154). Washington, DC: The Humane Society of the United States.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wemelsfelder, F., Haskell, M., Mendl, M., Calvert, S., & Lawrence, A. (2000). Diversity of behaviour during novel object tests is reduced in pigs housed in substrate-impoverished conditions. Animal Behaviour, 60, 385–394. doi:10.1006/anbe.2000.1466

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, D. (1998). Adaptive individual differences within single populations. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 353, 199–205.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, D., Clark, A., Coleman, K., & Dearstyne, T. (1994). Shyness and boldness in humans and other animals. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 9, 442–446.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, D. S., Coleman, K., Clark, A. B., & Biederman, L. (1993). Shy-bold continuum in pumpkinseed sunfish (Lepomis gibbosus): An ecological study of a psychological trait. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 107, 250.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Würbel, H., Bergeron, R., & Cabib, S. (2006). The coping hypothesis of stereotypic behaviour. Box 3, Chapter 1. In G. Mason & J. Rushen (Eds.), Stereotypic animal behaviour: Fundamentals and applications to welfare (pp. 14–15). Wallingford: CAB International.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Malin K. Lilley .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2017 Springer International Publishing AG

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Lilley, M.K., Kuczaj, S.A., Yeater, D.B. (2017). Individual Differences in Nonhuman Animals: Examining Boredom, Curiosity, and Creativity. In: Vonk, J., Weiss, A., Kuczaj, S. (eds) Personality in Nonhuman Animals. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-59300-5_13

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics